Critical Thinking Mendel

Abstract

The described interdisciplinary course helped a mixed population of in-service secondary English and biology teacher-participants increase their genetics content knowledge and awareness of Ethical, Legal, and Social Implications (ELSI) that arose from discoveries and practices associated with the Human Genome Project. This was accomplished by applying a critical literacy approach that allows people develop cognitive skills such that they are able to “read the world” (Wink, 2004). The approach is one that permits readers to go beyond the literal text to examine what is present as well as what is missing as it relates to issues of equity and fairness. Becoming critically literate enabled these teacher-participants to challenge the subtle attitudes, values, and beliefs conveyed by a range of written and oral texts. The teacher-participants in this course improved their critical literacy skills by actively reading, critically writing about, and using evidence to support their conclusions about issues arising from advances in human genetics. A biologist, a linguist, and an educator collaboratively designed and taught the course. The personalized focus on the integration of thoughtful reading and writing in this class enhanced the teacher-participants' (n = 16) professional and intellectual development and will potentially improve learning in their biology and English classrooms in the future.

INTRODUCTION

When we each participated in the ELSI (Ethical, Legal, and Social Implications) summer institutes at Dartmouth University we were struck by the intricate implications of the Human Genome Project (HGP) and the possibilities for creating a highly-motivating learning experience with the topic. We also realized that as different content area specialists—molecular biology, linguistics, and teacher educator with a background in history and philosophy of science —teaching at a public liberal arts institution the topic would lend itself to an interdisciplinary approach to which we could each contribute. So in the fall of 2005 we searched for an important developmental platform from which to stage the intended outcomes of the course. We settled upon critical literacy, the ability and stance of readers to interrogate the implicit “statements” of a text, because we believed that as teachers' critical literacy skills increased so too was the likelihood that they could positively promote the critical literacy skills of their students. Only with the citizenry's analytical skills solidly in hand can a democracy thrive. We proposed a course entitled “Critical Literacy in Genetics: Ethical and Social Implications of the Human Genome Project” to be collaboratively taught June, 2006.

History and Design of the Course

Two of us had previously applied our ELSI experiences as we taught summer/fall courses in 2002 and 2003 for secondary life science teachers, which provided some of the insights for the development of this course (Gleason and Kleine, 2003). From those classroom experiences we learned significant lessons as to how to close the gap between the broad implications heralded by cutting-edge genetics and the negligible knowledge base the average citizen, or in our case, biology teacher, had accessible for this focus. We knew that the topic was a highly-engaging one and that it could be understood from many perspectives, but after teaching two iterations to in-service science teachers we also wanted to do more to prod them to consider the worth of what they were learning. From these science courses we learned that the participants acquired important knowledge of molecular genetics (e.g., transformation and gene expression) and, to a lesser extent through the class discussion component that we incorporated, developed an appreciation for the HGP's ethical ramifications. Our evaluations also informed us that the teachers augmented their critical thinking skills, a positive result. The participants in these earlier offerings also discovered some good hands-on activities that they could apply in their life science classrooms. However, we sought some greater personal effect on teacher-participants' everyday lives than we had been able to imbue previously, a change in disposition toward the value of thinking critically as well as enhancement of their ability to apply skepticism to the act of “reading the world” (Wink, 2004).

As the three of us came together to create a course for 2006 we reflected on our own summer experiences at the ELSI Institutes. We realized that there we had been expected to reconcile the consequences of the HGP that provide advantages to individuals and society concomitantly with its disadvantages. We performed an ongoing mental calculus that was enriched by the speaking, reading, writing, viewing, and discussing that we did in community. In other words, in the ELSI workshops we built our understanding through small group work and roundtables of the whole to continually assess whether the benefits wrought by the HGP were worth the drawbacks and if so, under what circumstances. To question, to wonder, to assert, to defend, and to persuade all seemed to be powerful elements of our own learning experiences. We wanted a similarly empowering and intellectually stimulating experience for the teacher-participants we planned to teach. Thus we looked for a greater imperative for the teacher-participants to learn about the HGP than merely to pass it on to others. As we learned about critical literacy we determined that it could provide the pivot point for a course that could offer empowering and intellectual outcomes. Furthermore critical literacy aligned well with the skeptical attitude and logic skills necessary to engage in scientific thinking. Thus we concluded that an interdisciplinary course for teaching about the HGP that developed the critical literacy stance of the participants was a good meld of our aptitudes and shared goals. Moreover, our goal was not only to improve the critical literacy of our students (teacher-participants), but also through our teacher-participants to impact the teaching and literacy levels of their middle grades and high-school English and biology students.

Critical Literacy

We took a critical literacy approach in this class. This is not a single instructional strategy as much as a means to assist readers in adopting a skeptical, analytical mindset, not unlike that used by a trained scientist in her work. Because critical literacy enables one to challenge the subtle attitudes, values, and beliefs conveyed by a range of written and oral “texts” (Alvermann and Hagood, 2000) group discussion of shared readings is the most effective way to open texts to multiple interpretations and “teach” this concept. Critical literacy has its roots in the pedagogical theories of Paulo Freire (1970) who examined how changes could be effected in the lives of the Brazilian oppressed by having them learn to read and write and then to use those literacy skills to actively question those in power. As noted by Taylor (1993), Freire argued for raising learners' consciousness through discussion and writing and felt that their learning had to come out of their lived experiences. When a critically literate person engages with a text she would be examining issues of language, power, equality, justice, and society. A critically literate teacher would go beyond these issues to reflect on his own teaching, reading, and writing. One proponent, Wink (2004), has likened critical literacy to “… learning to read the world”. Because the HGP holds countless propositions for current and future worlds, which cannot necessarily be taken at face value, we believed that teaching the participants to be critically literate would enable them to scrutinize the implications and call them into question, understanding that no text is neutral and all knowledge is situated.

The notion of critical literacy that emerged from the work of Paolo Freire as he advocated for adult Brazilians to question the status quo ultimately concerns itself with identity formation (Freire, 1970). The term has come to have many nuances, but we were guided by a narrower one that Simpson (1996) used in teaching middle-school teachers to read and write, namely that texts reflects a particular view and as such do not necessarily correspond to “reality.” Simpson also noted that authors are influential because they write in such a way so as to position the reader to respond to a text in particular ways: with the selection of the language they use, the point of view they take, and other similarly dynamic choices they make when drafting pieces. Our goal was to enable the participants to more actively question texts so that they would read and write in such a way as to reflect their understanding of this power of their growing literacy skills.

For this course we elected to expand the meaning of literacy to include both the traditional notion of reading and writing competence as well as the more current understanding of this term as applied in science (Freeman and Taylor, 2006). According to the National Science Education Standards, “scientific literacy means that a person can ask, find, or determine answers to questions derived from curiosity about everyday experiences. It entails being able to read with understanding articles about science in the popular press and to engage in social conversation about the validity of the conclusions. Scientific literacy implies that a person can identify scientific issues underlying national and local decisions and express positions that are scientifically and technologically informed. A literate citizen should be able to evaluate the quality of scientific information on the basis of its source and the methods used to generate it. Scientific literacy also implies the capacity to pose and evaluate arguments based on evidence and to apply conclusions from such arguments appropriately” (National Research Council, 1995, emphasis ours). In other words, to be scientifically literate carries with it several capacities—dispositional, procedural, and cognitive—that one would apply in school and beyond. What language literacy and scientific literacy have in common then is the sense that increased competence in either advances one's intellectual development and ability to carry out systematic thinking that involves abstraction. In Freire's assessment of traditional literacy instruction he recognized the liberating aspect that literacy and critical thinking, which he called “thought-language,” entailed. In fact he referred to the process of teaching adults to read and write as “cultural action for freedom” and understood that the outcome of being critically literate didn't change merely one person but positively affected the entire culture (Freire, 1970). Our thought was that we could join the advancement of both scientific and language literacies together as “critical literacies” to enhance and develop the participants' knowledge, and ultimately their students' knowledge, of and in both areas.

Through our course we sought to offer the teacher-participants meaningful professional development that would have their personal and social transformation as its end result (Loucks-Horsley et al., 2003). By making critical literacy the focal point we could choose and assign texts about the HGP anticipating that teacher-participants would grapple with its implications at an elevated and transformative manner. We planned to have teacher-participants improve their critical literacy skills by discussing, actively reading on, critically writing about, and using evidence to support conclusions on issues arising from recent scientific discoveries. As one teacher-participant offered about her experience in learning to apply critical literacy, “I research, speak about, listen to, and understand the concepts that apply to writing about the various issues related to this topic” (A.T., emphasis ours). Although comments such as these indicate participants' active role in becoming increasingly critically literate, we plan to evaluate the degree of achievement in a future research project. The scope and duration of this project was such that we were unable to focus specifically on the depth of the transformative experiences, but instead chose to focus on the teacher-participants' improvement in their active reading and critical writing skills as it pertains to the HGP and the ethical, legal, and social implications of it.

Scientific Thinking

Our aim was to construct an interdisciplinary educational sequence as we highlighted the science of genomics throughout the course. Thus we searched for the values and methods that sustain the scientific enterprise to weave into the course on HGP and critical literacy. We identified characteristics of scientific thinking that were akin to those used by the critically literate, including the logic of scientific thinking and the skeptical attitude scientists use in their work. Logic allows one to make sound conclusions by relying on evidence, while skepticism asks one to resist accepting a belief without good reason as well as to be open-minded enough to hold beliefs tentatively. Looking back to Freire we posited that his “pedagogy of hope” would build reasoning skills such as are exercised in logic and would denounce gullibility as an oppressive tactic (Freire, 1970). We believe that Freire would agree that critical literacy parallels scientific thinking in many ways. Furthermore, recent cognitive research from investigators such as Donovan and Bransford (2005) suggests that teaching science in more socially constructivist ways can be more effective than the use of transmission models, therefore teaching strategies using small group work and discussion were frequently used. Equipped with the goal of promoting critical literacy in a collaboratively taught, interdisciplinary course that explored the HGP and associated issues, we felt that we could adequately tap into the development of scientific thinking at the same time. (Readers wishing to access some of the activities and assignments that were designed to promote the goals described in this essay as well as some of the assessment tools and data resulting from the evaluation of this study may do so at http://hercules.gcsu.edu/∼mgleason/teacherquality/06supplementarydata.htm.)

METHODOLOGY

The participants in the course were in-service teacher-participants from schools in central Georgia counties. Eight of them took the course for graduate biology credit while the same number enrolled for English credit. The class was conducted for six hours daily for eight days over two weeks. The assigned texts, Genome by Matt Ridley (2000) and assorted readings from the ELSI Institute and other popular and academic texts were sent in advance so that teacher-participants could prepare for the intense demands of the short course. Teacher-participants were assessed using two pre- and postinstruments, (an essay and a concept map), given before instruction and again on the last day to provide measures of learning gains. The essays were used to gauge teacher-participant awareness of the HGP, while the concept maps measured the complexity of teacher-participants' critical thinking skills that developed as they assimilated new knowledge. Three summative formal essays were used as a principle means of grading the teacher-participants in the course. Students were given rubrics to guide their writing of these essays. The first two of the essays have also been evaluated via a separate assessment rubric. Teacher-participants engaged in other formative and summative assessments such as “minute papers” about the direction of the course, peer editing and review sessions, classroom discussions received written feedback from instructors on essays on topics they selected and investigated, and made self-assessments of developing skills throughout the course (e.g., journaling). These assessment techniques allowed us to evaluate nuances of teacher-participant learning that the pre- and postassessments could not as readily address. Various assessment tools, rubrics, or schemes used to analyze the teacher-participant works can be found at the website noted above along with additional evaluative data and some materials from the course.

While the format of each day varied, nearly every day included an intensive scientific investigation and writing period. This entailed direct instruction from the professors (authors of this paper) and supplementary content support from two graduate assistants, one of whom had expertise in genetics and the other who was well-grounded in communication and critical literacy. It also included discussion in large and small groups, (often in relation to a case study), and questions from professors directed at scaffolding teacher-participants' connections among personal, professional, pedagogical, and scientific issues relating to the HGP. We designed this rich interplay of learner-centered activities based on the constructivist principles of Tharp and Gallimore (1989), along with our personal experience with such principles, with the intent that it would foster rapid assimilation of the HGP content and the critical literacy skills needed to address its issues. We also believed that these activities would become models that the teacher-participants would implement in their own classrooms so that the students they worked with might develop critical literacy skills as well. Participants received feedback on their written work; in addition, they completed peer and self-assessments to stimulate their thinking about any changes being wrought. From the learning activities mentioned above we selected a range of formats—written, visual, and oral discussion—as the basis for our interpretations.

Pre- and postessays (informal writings) were quantified by counting the number of times a teacher-participant wrote about a distinct HGP fact or issue. Factual errors were counted as well. A scholarly essay rubric was used to quantify the first two formal writings of the teacher-participants. Similar to the criteria included on the rubric they used to guide their writing, we applied two broad categories to assess their argumentation and substantiation skills by examining their “reflections on societal impacts” and their genetics comprehension by quantifying and scoring the quality of their distinct understandings of the “relationship between Mendelian and molecular genetics” and their use of “genetic nomenclature.” Concept maps were scored by a scheme to quantify critical thinking. This scheme assigned points for the complexity of each of the ideas expressed (low: more concepts than interconnections identified within the concept map [1] or high: more interconnections than concepts identified within a concept map [2]) as well as the number of multiple connections, multiple levels, or additional self-selected concepts indicated on a concept map. While we created our own concept map scoring scheme, it is similar to those used by others in science education to find a general measure of the depth of cognitive change that might be occurring (Novak, 1991). The changes we expected in the informal writings and essays included the participants' use of stronger arguments based on evidence from concepts addressed in the course, more robust use of logic skills, and to a lesser degree, improvement in mechanics and usage. Regarding the concept maps, we expected teacher-participants to identify a greater number of genomic concepts (nodes) and indicate more relationships between concepts (propositions) as a result of their learning. Additionally, we hoped to see teacher-participants increase the number of levels to include concepts, subconcepts, and details. All of these features would contribute to what we termed complexity. We used direct quotations from their written output as a part of the documentation of the qualitative outcomes.

Findings

The ultimate aim was for participants' to adopt a critical stance toward issues associated with the HGP and to extend this personally and professionally beyond the requirements of the course to assist in their intellectual development. The objectives included the acquisition of genetics content knowledge as well as skill in incorporating and then applying critical literacy strategies in science with middle and high school students. The participants were continually asked to examine the HGP for its consequent, and often, provocative issues throughout the course.

The data we address below include: 1) informal writing tasks which allowed us to document changes between the two pre- and postassessments (a writing assignment about HGP issues and a concept map on genetics knowledge), 2) formal essays written at the beginning and middle of the course, 3) the use of concept maps, which allowed the enumeration of issues—scientific, ethical, or social—identified by teacher-participants, 4) group discussions and critique of the film GATTACA (Nichol 1997), and 5) a final journal entry completed about the meaning and applicability of critical literacy. For this we directly quoted from the teacher-participants' papers.

Informal Writing

The same written assessment was conducted before the first day and on the last day of the course to be used to identify any learning changes. In a 30-min time period and without the use of reference materials teacher-participants were asked to write what they knew about the “Human Genome Project and associated genetic issues.” A simple quantification of distinct facts about the HGP, ELSI issues related to HGP, and incorrect statements is given in Table 1 for pre- and postassessments given for English teachers (n = 8) and biology teachers (n = 8). As expected, on the postassessment, all teacher-participants demonstrated dramatic gains in their awareness of the project's implications. More striking, though not unexpected, the eight English teacher-participants exhibited substantial gains in comparison to the eight biology teacher-participants, in their basic understanding of the science supporting the HGP.

Table 1.

Quantification of HGP facts, issues, and factual errors cited by English and science teacher-participants in their written preassessment and postassessment essays

Preassessment comments from English teacher-participants covered a considerable range. At the lower end, some teacher-participants professed essentially no knowledge to vague awareness such as: “[I have] little knowledge of the ‘genome project and associated genetic issues’ but I've read about a ‘cloned sheep’” and that “DNA has been very helpful to forensics workers” (D.W.). Another person felt that his knowledge about the “… HGP can fit inside a thimble” (B.G.). A third English teacher-participant wrote about Mendel and his “purple or white flowers, twirling or straight vines” (A.T.). Some English teacher-participants at the course's commencement described the science of the HGP using technical jargon, such as “genes consist of 23 pairs of chromosomes” (S.B.), or “isolating a part of a chromosome that is responsible for different things” (L.A.). By contrast, at the course's end, teacher-participants saw the HGP's science, albeit still imperfectly, with a more complete lens, that shed relevant light on their understanding of its implications: “The HGP is the effort by science to understand the story of each individual as revealed through his or her genes” (A.T., emphasis in original), “446ow that we have identified the components of the genome itself, what do we do with what is uncovered?” (S.B.), and “… ethical implications [of the HGP] involve determining what alleles are acceptable to select … and [the limits of] reproductive freedom/control” (L.A.). Finally, one English teacher-participant who came in with a “thimbleful” of knowledge saw himself at the end of the course as a “mason jar now overflowing” with knowledge about the HGP (B.G.).

The biology teachers, on the other hand, exhibited a basic awareness of the HGP. In their initial essays, all but one correctly including historical information such as, “The HGP began in 1990” (W.J.) or, “It is attempting to identify the sequence of every base [pair] of DNA in the human genome” (V.Y.). Again, this is not surprising, because presentation of the history of the HGP is a component of the state's life science curriculum. Nevertheless, gains in genetic knowledge were made, as their postessays showed a firmer grasp of the genetic processes and biological science addressed in the course. One example included a high-school biology teacher with an enhanced discernment of the connection between linkage groups and polygenic traits, as well as the ability to distinguish between different forms of neonatal genetic testing, such as preimplantation genetic diagnosis (PGD) and amniocentesis (CG).

Formal Writing

In addition to being provided with reading materials before the class began, teacher-participants were provided an essay rubric detailing the requirements to be met for each paper; from this rubric we created an essay scoring rubric aligned with those requirements. After intense discussion of the issues under consideration on the first and second days of class, teacher-participants were asked to research and write about “the right of the parent to choose a child's traits.” Similarly, on the fourth and fifth days of class they were asked to write about “genetic discrimination.” We codified the gains evidenced in these writings by assessing genetics/HGP knowledge and ability to research, explicate, and substantiate that knowledge. Instances of each of these rubric-scored measures are presented as overall measures of “argumentation and substantiation” and a “combined genetics understanding and nomenclature use” for both papers and comparing the English teacher-participants to science teacher-participants (Figure 1). The data largely matched expectations: there were gains on the parts of both the English and the science teacher-participants in terms of knowledge of the HGP and associated terms and issues, although the gains were greater on the part of the English teacher-participants. Specifically, a better understanding of a basic relationship between molecular and Mendelian genetics was exhibited, as was an increased comfort with the nomenclature of genomics. This was shown in their writing by employing such terms as 10q26 (a locus region on a chromosome) and gene name symbols (e.g., LEPR or ENGRAILED). One unexpected result, however, was the slight decrease in English teacher-participants' facility in creating meaningful propositions (completeness and coherence) to support their arguments on the second paper, as is shown in Figure 2. These findings were obtained from examination of the subscores for “argumentation and substantiation.” Our explanation for this is that, although their writing skills were polished insofar as discussing literature and doing literary analysis, there was some cognitive dissonance created when they were confronted by completely new terms and ideas. Recent publications in a special Science issue dedicated to the language, literacy, and science (American Association for the Advancement of Science, 2010) suggest this proposition is one that we could explore in future research. Even with this slight discrepancy on the part of the English participants, however, it was apparent that the quantity and quality of each teacher-participant's written assessment tasks became richer and more detailed; overall, they demonstrated a critical, thoughtful approach to the issues raised in class. Additionally, most of the overall science teacher-participants improvements in “argumentation and substantiation” seemed attributable to gains in their documentation skills. A table presenting the rubric scores used in this analysis is found on the accompanying website (see http://hercules.gcsu.edu/∼mgleason/teacherquality/06supplementarydata.htm).

Figure 1.

Overall rubric scores for the first and second essays are presented for the English teacher-participants (striped bars) and the science teacher-participants (solid bars). Error bars show SE of the mean.

Figure 2.

Essay 1 and 2 rubric-subscores for argumentation and substantiation skill measure are presented for the English teacher-participants (striped bars) and the science teacher-participants (solid bars).

Concept Maps

In general we anticipated that the biology teachers would have more content knowledge about genomics before instruction while the English teachers would have better writing skills. Although this supposition generally held it was not true in all cases, as three teachers registered in the biology section taught Earth or physical science and had less knowledge of genomics than did the other science teachers in the course. The participants were not told the main concept around which to arrange their maps but were asked to include 12 prescribed terms when constructing the map. However, they were encouraged to include additional ones if they could.

Six of the eight science teachers organized all 12 terms correctly if superficially on the initial concept map. One was incomplete, using only three terms, and one was organized in no coherent manner and not documented on the accompanying website. The biology participants selected terms that suggested a more global approach to the task such as inheritance or genetics, or tended to apply one of their own as the organizing concept for the map, whereas the English teachers frequently used the term (one that they may have heard in popular media) DNA, as the organizing concept. Two high school teachers added many more terms than the 12 provided and one middle school science teacher was the most elaborate in establishing relationships between concepts, also called propositions, on her map. Although instructed to do otherwise, five of the eight science teachers provided few (< 3) to no propositions on their concept maps in advance of the course. Example of English and science teacher-participants pre- and post-concept maps are shown in Figure 3. All concept maps and their individual scores can be seen at the accompanying website (http://hercules.gcsu.edu/∼mgleason/teacherquality/06supplementarydata.htm).

Figure 3.

(A and B) Pre- and postconcepts maps for an English teacher participant that improved in complexity score from a 2 to a 5. (C and D) Science teacher-participant that improved from 2 to 4.

The maps constructed by the English teachers' for the most part revealed little initial knowledge of genetics. As a whole their understanding was below the basic level. Only one person included propositions, one added many additional terms but they were ethical rather than life science concepts, and only two selected truly central terms such as inheritance around which to organize the map. Two were so minimal as to not be open to our analysis of their conceptions.

The concept maps completed as a postassessment instrument were noticeably better developed, in terms of complexity for essentially every teacher-participant. Granted, the teacher-participants had a small amount of instruction on how to use concept maps as assessment tools that may have led to their improved construction. However, the instruction provided little more information than they were given in the original instructions such as to be explicitly told to include propositions to indicate relationships between concepts. An example of a postassessment concept map drawn by one of the English teacher-participants is shown in Figure 1, and the scored results are available on the website. Every class member arranged all 12 terms, including mitochondrial DNA, in a logical manner, and all but two provided propositions that established accurate relationships between concepts. Ten teachers-participants, six science and four English, used sophisticated propositions (three or more words in the phrase used to create more than a simple predicate) to denote the relationships among concepts with six actually showing two or more relationships with a single concept demonstrating increased complexity. Fifty percent of the participants included more subconcepts on the postassessment than required, and many were creative in their choice of the organizing concept. Two English teacher-participants organized around the term HGP, one around genetic discrimination, and one around cells, suggesting overall heightened, more global insights, and a much richer conception than they demonstrated originally. The map of one person, initially at the lowest level of conceptual understanding, remained so, but generally we think the concept maps revealed that the learners developed a much more solid knowledge base with a more advanced facility to consider the concepts at a high level or with more complexity.

Group Discussion and Film Critique

Discussion was a frequent strategy throughout the course, at times facilitated by the professors with particular questions used as prompts. While the data described in this section are anecdotal, we noticed several interesting patterns that indicated our teacher-participants were indeed learning more about issues related to the HGP. In comparing participants' oral responses to directed-questioning during early and late discussions in the course, we found that almost all the science teachers showed detailed understanding of reproductive, biological, and genetic technologies, by the end of the course. Similarly, more-detailed responses were also exhibited by at least half of the English teachers by the end of the course, though their robust understanding of each technology was more limited than the life science teachers' responses. One example of the subtleties in discernment that the teachers' displayed included the ability to discriminate neonatal genetic testing technologies, such as PGD and single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) analysis, from assisted-reproduction technologies such as hyperovulation and in vitro fertilization. Going from that seemingly ‘simple’ analysis earlier in the course to a more sophisticated ability later on, in which teacher-participants distinguished these two categories from biological discoveries in the fields of cloning and embryonic stem cell studies, we felt, was a triumph for all concerned.

Similarly, we saw interesting responses when participants critiqued the science fiction film GATTACA both at the beginning of the course and after eight days of instruction. We asked them to list incidences they saw that they would identify as science and biotechnology, as well as issues they would place in the legal, social, or ethical implications category. We noticed that teacher-participants were able to identify more such issues upon the second viewing, which we believe reflected the depth to which they came to understand both scientific concepts as well as ELSI issues. As one teacher-participant wrote, “It was also much more poignant because I realize how truly close it is to reality and not fantasy” (A.J., emphasis in the original).

Quotations from the Final Journal Entry

In the final writing assignment participants were asked to define critical literacy, how they had been using it in the course, and how they might use the HGP and the issues it evokes to promote critical literacy. As one biology teacher wrote, “[C]ritical literacy means reading,” but then elaborated that it involves multiple processes (reading, viewing, discussing, documentation, and writing) with critical questioning, “[I]s the author knowledgeable on this subject? Do they know enough to write about it? … present a good case for their point of view? Are you convinced by their documentation that they are correct in their stand and if not, why not?” He then went on to write “… after this [critical analysis] the person can now formulate their own position … based on clear evaluation of material and not just emotions” (J.D.). Another added that critical literacy takes students to the next level of library research, because too often they “… are not made aware that they can question sources” (S.B.). As regards use of critical literacy during the course, P.C. typified many when she wrote, “I am applying critical literacy by reflecting on issues concerning all of us. By examining issues that affect us, I now ask myself, how does the knowledge promote the health and well being of individuals in a society?” With reference to the use of HGP as a pretext for teaching critical literacy, J.P., an English high school teacher, wrote, “I would definitely use GATTACA as … a launch-pad for discussion of both ‘literary’ elements like symbolism [and] more scientific themes [like] what a gene is, what its function is, how does the HGP, gene therapy, and genetic selection have political, social, and cultural impacts …” In addition, he added, “I would use external reading material … like Ridley's Genome … and then have them write about their impressions.” Some of the teacher-participants were in agreement that the subject matter we used was effective in promoting critical literacy. Others indicated that more succinct accounts, such as newspaper articles that we introduced from daily newspapers published during the course, would be more accessible to their students and add the relevancy of ‘current events’ (J.D.). Finally, several participants felt as one learner did, that the course made her, “feel more secure in discussing this material with my students” (VY). Participant reflections reveal an understanding of how critical literacy can be used effectively in multidisciplinary courses.

Final Lessons Learned

We felt the course was successful in many ways because the participants appeared to advance a great deal in a short period of time. We attributed the participants' enhanced professional and intellectual development to the personalized focus on thoughtful reading and writing they encountered with us throughout the course. We intend to follow up with more study to determine if indeed, teacher-participants improve in their ability to share their knowledge of the HGP with future generations and anecdotal data have been encouraging but this will take more resources than we have available at this time. Additionally, we anticipate that the modeling, instruction, and expectation for implementation provided through the community of discourse will strengthen the teachers' ability to bring all of their students to high levels of achievement. Many of the teacher-participants felt that meaningful activities they had utilized to explore social, ethical, and legal implications of the HGP, would increase their ability to elicit the enthusiasm of their students for biology and literature. For example, the biology teachers benefited from discussions, reading, and writing exercises that opened their minds to the implication of the HGP and spoke earnestly of trying to find ways to incorporate such activities into their classrooms. While the English teachers benefited from these activities as well, it was in the context of controversy that the science that they learned took on an even greater importance. All of the English teachers felt they would now be comfortable working with students writing essays like they wrote in class and some looked forward to tackling literature dealing with such subjects in their classroom as result of this course. We are hopeful that the teacher-participants have taken with them a more critical stance on all areas of science and life and will guide their students to do so as well.

In many of our courses we do not always feel we have the liberty or time to teach in this more responsive style. In this situation, however, we found the experience freeing as colleagues and teacher-participants from quite diverse fields, and our interactions increased our personal and professional growth. One measure of this was the degree of responsiveness and flexibility that we developed on a daily basis as this course evolved. Another was the many insights into literacy improvements we have described. In toto, we believe these factors have been and will be put in place in all of our future courses, and in many of the classrooms of the teachers who took this class.

This innovative course challenged us, the instructors, to communicate in many areas, such as integrating our teaching methods, materials, and ways of assessment. Although we met for many planning sessions before the course, we found that we had had to frequently assess and respond to a changing learning environment. We consulted with each other and with the graduate assistants throughout the day and more extensively almost each afternoon in debriefing sessions. Besides discussing technical and logistical considerations, we had to learn how each of us addressed issues associated with the topics that had arisen in class about the HGP and what we believed our teacher-participants were gaining from the course. Hence, we recommend that as other instructors design and implement interdisciplinary courses that they make it part of their plan to set aside time to communicate during the course as well. As a learning experience we calculate this to have been beneficial for all involved and expect it to be similarly successful for the learning of middle and high school students.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank the dedicated service of Spencer Pucci and Jennifer Lindenberger, whose assistance helped the course run smoothly. Funding for the course described in this paper was provided by a “No Child Left Behind” Title II Part A Higher Education Improving Teacher Quality Higher Education Grant from the U.S. Department of Education (2006-1SLRB3) and the GCSU Science Education Center. Courses previously taught by Drs. Gleason and Kleine were also funded the U.S. Department of Education under the auspices of Eisenhower Higher Education Grant Program (2002-03SD2) and the Improving Teacher Quality Grant Program (2003-1SD6).

REFERENCES

Alvermann and Hagood, 2000. Alvermann D. E., Hagood M. C. Critical media literacy: research, theory, and practice in “New Times.” J. Educ. Res. 2000;93:193–205.

American Association for the Advancement of Science, 2010. American Association for the Advancement of Science. Science. 2010;328

Donovan and Bransford, 2005. Donovan S. M., Bransford J. D. How Students Learn: History, Mathematics, and Science in the Classroom. In: Donovan Susan M., editor. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2005.

Freeman and Taylor, 2006. Freeman G., Taylor V. Integrating Science and Literacy Instruction: A Framework for Bridging the Gap. New York: Rowman and Littlefield Education; 2006.

Freire, 1970. Freire P. The adult literacy process as cultural action for freedom. Harvard Educ. Rev. 1970;40:205–212.

Gleason and Kleine, 2003. Gleason M. L., Kleine K.L.M. Teaching GATTACA, Poster 1384 presented at the 43rd Annual Meeting of American Society for Cell Biology; San Francisco, CA (ASCB). 2003. p. 106.

Loucks-Horsley et al., 2003. Loucks-Horsley S., Love N., Stiles K. E., Mundry S. E., Hewson P. W. Designing Professional Development for Teachers of Science and Mathematics. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press; 2003.

National Research Council, 1995. National Research Council. National Science Education Standards: Observe, Interact, Change, Learn. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 1995.

Nichol, 1997. Nichol A. United States: Columbia Tristar Home Entertainment. 1997:2002. (writer and director of GATTACA DVD)

Novak, 1991. Novak J. Concept-mapping: a useful tool for science education [Electronic version] J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1991;36:475–492.

Ridley, 2000. Ridley M. Genome: An Autobiography in of a Species in 23 Chapters. New York: HarperCollins Publishers; 2000.

Simpson, 1996. Simpson A. Critical questions: whose questions? The Reading Teacher. 1996;50:118–127.

Taylor, 1993. Taylor P. V. The Texts of Paulo Freire. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press; 1993.

Tharp and Gallimore, 1989. Tharp R. G., Gallimore R. G. Rousing Minds to Life: Teaching, Learning, and Schooling in Social Context. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1989.

Wink, 2004. Wink J. Critical Pedagogy: Notes from the Real World. 3rd ed. New York: Allyn and Bacon; 2004.

Gregor Mendel's classic paper and the nature of science in genetics courses

Authors

  • Julie F. Westerlund,

    1. Texas State University, San Marcos, TX, USA
    Search for more papers by this author
  • Daniel J. Fairbanks

    1. Texas State University, San Marcos, TX, USA
    Search for more papers by this author

Julie F. Westerlund, 601 University Drive, Texas State University, San Marcos, TX 78666, USA. E-mail: jw33@txstate.edu

Abstract

The discoveries of Gregor Mendel, as described by Mendel in his 1866 paper Versuche über Pflanzen-Hybriden (Experiments on plant hybrids), can be used in undergraduate genetics and biology courses to engage students about specific nature of science characteristics and their relationship to four of his major contributions to genetics. The use of primary source literature as an instructional tool to enhance genetics students’ understanding of the nature of science helps students more clearly understand how scientists work and how the science of genetics has evolved as a discipline. We offer a historical background of how the nature of science developed as a concept and show how Mendel's investigations of heredity can enrich biology and genetics courses by exemplifying the nature of science.

“One can no longer teach genetics only as genetics but must teach genetics also as a science. The reason for this is that our nation and the world seem to be pitted against an ever-increasing galaxy of problems that relate to our interactions with one another and with the natural world […] Whatever decisions are made depend on one's mode of thought and whether one thinks of today only or of the future as well.”

John Moore 1986, p. 573

These perceptive words of John Moore, a quarter of a century ago, concerning genetics courses and echoed more recently by Bruce Alberts, current Editor-in-Chief of the journal Science and past president of the Natural Academy of Sciences (NAS) in the United States, stress the need for undergraduate science courses that focus on the ‘nature of science and development of scientific knowledge’.

“An objective analysis of a typical introductory science course in colleges and universities around the world […] would probably conclude that its purpose [goal] is to prepare students to ‘know, use and interpret scientific explanations of the natural world […] The other three goals of equal merit are to prepare students to generate and evaluate scientific evidence and explanations, to understand the nature and development of scientific knowledge, and to participate productively in scientific practices and discourse. Scientists would generally agree that all four types of science understanding are critical not only to a good science education but also to the basic education of everyone in the modern world. Why then do most science professors teach only the first one?”

Bruce Alberts 2009

Science curricula that include the nature and development of scientific knowledge (nature of science) exist in many countries worldwide. Examples of nature of science content are embedded in curricula in the United States, United Kingdom, Canada, Denmark and Spain (Matthews 1998, 2000). Science agencies in the United States, such as the NAS, the National Science Foundation (NSF), the National Research Council (NRC) and the American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS), have promoted reform in basic science courses to ensure that undergraduate students are better able to understand such complex scientific issues as climate change, world hunger, human diseases, rainforest depletion, overpopulation, evolution and pollution. Thereby, they may influence rational decisions about national policies. The NRC stresses that science courses should not only include science content, but also an “understanding of the nature and structure of scientific knowledge and the process by which it is developed.” (NRC 2007, p. 168). For teachers of genetics, it may be daunting to expand an already crowded course schedule. In our genetics courses, we discuss the nature of science in the context of Mendel. And, as this paper demonstrates, the time-honored lecture topic concerning the work of Gregor Mendel, can be quite useful in illustrating the “nature and development of scientific knowledge.”

PURPOSE

The purpose of this essay is two-fold: 1) to offer a brief historical background concerning the development of the concept of the nature of science and its relevance to undergraduate science curricula and 2) to show how Mendel's investigations of heredity can enrich biology and genetics courses by exemplifying the nature of science.

NATURE OF SCIENCE (NOS)

The nature of science (NOS) concerns the actual practice of science and the development of scientific knowledge. The practice of science includes, the “scientific method,” but also extends to include all legitimate means for conducting scientific inquiry. Scientific inquiry is characterized by the AAAS (1989) as:

“Scientific inquiry is not easily described apart from the context of particular investigations. There simply is no fixed set of steps that scientists always follow, no one path that leads them unerringly to scientific knowledge. There are, however, certain features of science [nature of science] that give it a distinctive character as a mode of inquiry. Although those features are especially characteristic of the work of professional scientists, everyone can exercise them in thinking scientifically about many matters of interest in everyday life.”

AAAS 1989

The traditional scientific method was first formally described and promoted by philosopher Francis Bacon in the early 1600s (Gribben 2002). This method is based upon induction in which all ideas are gathered together by an investigator prior to the formation of a hypothesis. Deductions that can be tested are then made from the hypotheses. However, not all scientific discoveries and knowledge are created from the crafted step-wise process outlined by Bacon (McComas et al. 2000). In the 20th century, Karl Popper described science as having the criterion of falsifiability. He stated, “The criterion of falsifiability says that statements or systems of statements, in order to be ranked as scientific, must be capable of conflicting with possible, or conceivable, observations” (Popper 1963). A companion distinction of the falsifiability criterion is “nothing is ever proven true in science”. An extension of Popper's characterization of science was the idea of ‘Science as a way of knowing’ (Moore 1993; Union of Concerned Scientists 2010). This popular phrase embodies the idea that science is experimental and based on observations of the natural world and that science is founded on hypotheses/theories that are falsifiable and cannot be proven but can be confirmed or refuted. It also asserts that scientific knowledge is subject to revision and change (Moore 1993). The nature of science is an expression more commonly used today than ‘Science as way of knowing’ and is an expanded perspective of the practice of science. For further reading in this area, Abd-El-Khalick and Lederman (2000) provide a detailed history of the development of NOS through the twentieth century.

CHARACTERISTICS OF THE NATURE OF SCIENCE

Characteristics of NOS were originally described in the 1950s by philosophers of science James Conant and J. Bronowski (Kimball 1967) and others in a variety of disciplines in the philosophy of science, history of science and science education. Although there is not a specific ‘list’ of nature of science characteristics, there are specific characteristics that are commonly agreed upon by those in the field. Abd-El-Khalick et al. succinctly listed the characteristics of NOS:

“…scientific knowledge is tentative (subject to change); empirically based (based on and/or derived from observations of the natural world); subjective (theory-laden); partly the product of human inference, imagination, and creativity (involves the invention of explanation); and socially and culturally embedded. Two additional important aspects are the distinction between observations and inferences, and the functions of, and relationships between scientific theories and laws…. At this level of generality, virtually no disagreement exists among historians, philosophers, and science educators.”

Abd-El-Khalick et al. 1998, p. 418

Alters (1997) and McComas et al. (2000) reviewed commonly held NOS tenets or characteristics from various science education organizations and researchers. Most contain the general ideas expressed in the passage above. Furthermore, US national science education reform documents, such as ‘Science for all Americans’ (AAAS 1989) and the ‘National Science Education Standards’ (NRC 1996), emphasized most of these NOS characteristics. One fundamental characteristic of the nature of science is that science is not dogma. Dogma represents a system of beliefs that is authoritarian, difficult to dispute and often used in a religious context. It is most important to stress to students and to society that scientific knowledge is not dogmatic but rather durable with gradual modifications as understanding of natural phenomena grows. Further, that scientific knowledge is durable rather than tentative is an important distinction in the NOS characteristics. As stated by the AAAS:

“Although scientists reject the notion of attaining absolute truth and accept some uncertainty as part of nature, most scientific knowledge is durable. The modification of ideas, rather than their outright rejection, is the norm in science, as powerful constructs tend to survive and grow more precise and to become widely accepted. Continuity and stability are as characteristic of science as change is, and confidence is as prevalent as tentativeness.”

AAAS 1989

For the benefit of our genetics courses, we have distilled the reported NOS characteristics into six major nature of science characteristics. These six NOS characteristics should help guide instructors and students in their own understanding of NOS in relation to science history. The following NOS characteristics need not be memorized by students, but serve as ‘tools’ when examining the history of science and development of scientific knowledge. The six NOS characteristics are that scientific knowledge: (1) may be considered as scientific laws or theories according to the type of knowledge (i.e. mathematical and/or explanatory), (2) is based upon evidence from observations of the natural world, (3) is embedded in the culture in which science is conducted, 4) is developed within prevailing scientific concepts (theory-laden observations and interpretations) and within scientists’ values, knowledge, and prior experiences, (5) originates from imaginative and creative processes, and lastly, (6) is not dogmatic but considered durable and is to be modified or replaced as further evidence requires, (inherently tentative) (Lederman et al. 2002; Abd-El-Khalick et al. 1998; AAAS 1989; NRC 1996, 2000). Hereafter, we will refer to these six characteristics as NOS-1 through NOS-6.

VIEWS OF SCIENTISTS ON THE NATURE OF SCIENCE

Those who teach undergraduate science courses, often practicing scientists, should be familiar with the characteristics of NOS so they are able to engage their students in discussions concerning it. As stated by the late Stephen Jay Gould:

“Science is inevitably socially embedded. As a practicing scientist, I believe, as we must, that there is an external truth out there. I also believe that science bumbles along fitfully towards knowledge of that external reality. And that is socially embedded and is inevitably so because it is done by human beings and not robots.

Gould 1987

“Nature is objective, and nature is knowable, but we can only view her through a glass darkly – and many clouds upon our vision are of our own making; social and cultural biases, psychological preferences, and mental limitations in universal modes of thought…”

Gould 1996, p. 8

In this section, we highlight views concerning the nature of science held by practicing scientists in order that the reader may see how the views of practicing scientists correspond to the list of six nature of science characteristics.

Early studies (Kimball 1967; Pomeroy 1993) of scientists’ understanding of the nature of science used survey methods and indicated limited understanding of the nature of science. To illustrate, Pomeroy (1993) studied 71 scientists from various disciplines and found that many of them expressed traditional views concerning the practice of science. Their views of science were characterized by strict objectivity and observation-based evidence.

More recently, Schwartz and Lederman (2008) interviewed 24 veteran research scientists from four different disciplines who had an average of 25 years of active research. Concerning the differences between theories and laws (NOS-1), more than one-half of the scientists held an hierarchical view that theories will develop into laws with time or more testing. The hierarchical view of theories and laws is a commonly held misconception. This misconception implies that scientific theories are not as well supported by evidence as scientific laws. It is one reason why the general public in the United States has resisted the theory of evolution although this theory is based on an enormous body of evidence. Unlike most hierarchical beliefs held by the public, scientists recognized that scientific laws may evolve as further evidence is accrued. (Schwartz and Lederman 2008).

The majority of these scientists acknowledged that the development and justification of scientific knowledge is based upon evidence from observation (NOS-2), although a few indicated that scientific knowledge could be based on strictly theoretical or mathematical ideas (for example, Albert Einstein's “thought experiments” that led to the theory of relativity) (Schwartz and Lederman 2008).

The majority of scientists (15) felt that today the direction of science is largely determined by funding and grants and, because of that; it is embedded in the culture (NOS-3). As one surveyed scientist indicated,

“You try to match your research idea with the funding agency. Sometimes you put a spin on it, or rationale that will convince the funding agency you want to work on a problem here that has implications to these problems the agency has interest in … Just doing research for research sake is gone … You have to be flexible with what you are willing to research. Be more dynamic and responsive or else you are not going to make it.

Schwartz and Lederman 2008 p. 753

Fifteen of the 24 scientists reported that their “theoretical framework, or that of other scientists” or current scientific theories determined the questions asked in their investigations and how they interpret their data (NOS-4). Schwartz and Lederman (2008, p. 747) stated that the scientists, “recognized theory-laden observations and investigations from within their research contexts and provided examples.”

Although the majority (16) of the surveyed scientists felt that scientific knowledge was partly created by reasoning and not solely from empirical data, several indicated that creativity was only involved in data interpretation and in design (NOS-5). To illustrate, one scientist remarked,

“You just look at the data and, you know, … interpretation … there is a lot of art … in the interpretation, there is a lot of creativity in how you choose to interpret the data, as well as in how you choose to design the experiment in the first place”

Schwartz and Lederman 2008, p. 751

Three of the scientists indicated that creativity is restricted to the initial stages of research during the design (Schwartz and Lederman 2008).

Of these scientists, only 46% acknowledged that scientific knowledge is inherently tentative (NOS-6). Scientists differed in their ideas concerning the certainty of scientific knowledge. The following illustrate differing views.

“An atmospheric scientist responded, when asked about the certainty of the model of the atom, “Certain. It's the way nature is” This view of certainty is in contrast to one of the theoretical physicists who stated in reference to scientists understanding of the atom, “As certain as we can be”

Schwartz and Lederman 2008, p. 742

“The aquatic ecologist commented: Scientific knowledge changes as better approximations of nature are realized, while religious knowledge is dependent on established (or accepted) elements …All scientific knowledge is subject to question, doubt and criticism (a further distinction from religion) … Nonetheless, someone will eventually challenge an accepted scientific finding and take a fresh look at it. … That is the self-corrective nature of science. Does science lead to universal truths? It leads to close approximations of universal truths.”

Schwartz and Lederman 2008, p. 742

RELEVANCE OF NATURE OF SCIENCE INSTRUCTION IN SCIENCE COURSES

Students in courses that are taught NOS in an explicit and reflective manner develop scientific literacy (Rutherford and Ahlgren 1991). Indicators of a scientifically literate person include: “A good understanding of basic scientific terms, concepts, and facts; an ability to comprehend how science generates and assesses evidence; and a capacity to distinguish science from pseudoscience” (Science Engineering Indicators p. 7–17). As Maienschein noted,

“Increased scientific literacy …produces skeptical habits of mind to keep seeking to know more and a willingness to accept change and revision. What is known one day may be replaced the next day with something quite different and even apparently contradictory. Scientific literacy teaches us to expect such change and difference, and gives us approaches for sorting through and selecting alternative accounts.… Science is useful in particular ways as a basis for informed decision making.”

Maienschein 1999, p. 83

By studying science history, such as Mendelian history, and the development of scientific knowledge, undergraduate students increase their understanding of how science is practiced, i.e. the nature of science, and their critical thinking skills. As Matthews (1998, p. 169) stated: “introductory philosophical analysis [study of nature of science] allows greater appreciation of the distinct empirical and conceptual issues involved when, for instance, Boyle's law, Dalton's model, or Darwin's theory is discussed. It also promotes critical and reflective thinking”.

Ignorance of science has serious consequences. Not being scientifically literate, and thus not being able to understand the evidence of global warming, may prevent efforts to abate catastrophic climatologic effects, such as rising sea levels, inundation of islands and shorelines, and severe weather and droughts worldwide. As Matthews stated, “The ability to distinguish good science from parodies and pseudoscience depends on a grasp of the nature of science. (Matthews 2000, p. xiv) Those who are better educated in science are better equipped to understand the nature of scientific inquiry, to distinguish science from non-science, and to examine more carefully “research processes” when determining whether scientific assertions are valid (Science and Engineering Indicators 2010, p. 7–27, p. 7–35), and are more likely to make informed decisions about scientific issues (AAAS 1989; NRC 1996; Kenyon and Resier 2006). An example of this is the performance of US residents on the 2008 General Social Survey (GSS). Of those who correctly answered the questions concerning scientific inquiry on the 2008 GSS, 74% recognized that astrology as being “not at all scientific” or being a non-science. Of those who did not correctly answer the inquiry question, only 57% recognized astrology as “not at all scientific” or a non-science. Thus, an understanding of the nature of science was associated with the correct perception that astrology is not a science. This correlation has been shown on all six GSS surveys administered since 1995 (Science and Engineering Indicators 2010, Table 7–16).

To train students to be able to distinguish science from non-science, science courses for undergraduate students, must inculcate “fundamental knowledge of what science is, and what it is not [nature of science], along with some key concepts” stated Alan Lesher, AAAS CEO (AAAS 2009). This requires consideration of science courses that better prepare students to become world citizens who are able to make informed decisions on issues of science.

EFFECTIVE NATURE OF SCIENCE INSTRUCTION IN UNDERGRADUATE SCIENCE COURSES

The most effective way to teach nature of science is through an explicit reflective approach (Abd-El-Khalick and Lederman 2000; Bell 2001; Bartholomew et al. 2004, Matthews 1998). As Bell states

…. the scientists who teach college level science courses believe that students will pick up current conceptions of the nature of science by ‘osmosis’ [implicitly] by listening to lectures about science, engaging in discussions about science, or by ‘doing’ science, including hands-on, inquiry-based activities. Yet the nature of science is a complex topic, and students’ misconceptions about the nature of science have proven as resistant to change as their misconceptions about other science content.”

Bell 2001

An explicit approach is purposeful and “addresses the nature of science head-on” (Bell 2001). Just as the concepts of science content are explicitly taught in science courses, so should the concepts of the nature of science be explicitly taught. However, Matthews (1998) recommends that explicit teaching of the nature of science must not resemble “indoctrination” of specific concepts. Instead, he suggests that teachers should question their students and engage them in discussions. As Matthews states:

“At a most basic level any text or scientific discussion will contain terms such as law, theory, model, explanation, cause, truth, knowledge, hypothesis, confirmation, observation, evidence, idealization, time, space, fields, and species. Similarly history—minimally in the form of names such as Galileo, Newton, Boyle, Darwin, Mendel, Faraday, Volta, Dalton, Bohr, Einstein, and so on – is unavoidable. A professional teacher should be able to elaborate a little on these matters – Philosophy begins when students and teachers slow down the science lesson and ask what the above terms mean and what the conditions are for their correct use… Students and teachers can be encouraged to ask the philosopher's standard questions: What do you mean by… ? and How do you know … ? It is preferable for students genuinely to struggle to grasp the simple questions than just repeat popular nostrums, or their teacher's prejudices, about the complex questions.”

Matthews 1998, p. 168–169

Explicit historical approaches to teaching the nature of science may involve introducing students to a specific list of commonly held nature of science characteristics that can be illustrated through the history of science (Clough 2007; Metz et al. 2007; McComas 2008). As noted above, students should be given a list not to memorize but rather to engender reflection, questions, and discussion about the development of scientific knowledge. For example, in the case of Mendel, historical vignettes and class discussions have been used successfully to illustrate the nature of science (Lonsbury and Ellis 2002; Clough et al. 2007). Lonsbury and Ellis (2002) introduced Mendelian history in a high-school biology classroom through discussion of scientific inquiry as “thinking outside of the box,” with Mendel as the example of an original thinker. Also, in this study, students discussed why Mendel's paper went unnoticed until 1900 (Lonsbury and Ellis 2002). This study revealed that the history-integration group, which had discussed Mendelian history, significantly outperformed the normal-instruction group on a total ‘Nature of scientific knowledge scale’ (NSKS), which is based on nature-of-science characteristics. Clough et al. (2007) developed a historical short story about Mendel (<http://science-stories.org/>) that had questions embedded within the story to engage the students in thinking about the nature of science as they read. Clough et al. (2007) demonstrated that as a result of their reading and reflecting on Mendel's story, undergraduate biology students improved their understanding of the creative and subjective aspects of the nature of science, Clough (2007), like Matthews (1998), stressed the importance of asking questions rather than simply listing the nature-of-science characteristics. Clough (2007) stated: “the key [for nature of science instruction] is to explore the nature of science as questions, so that science teachers and students come to a deeper understanding of the nature of science. […] ‘tenets’ can easily be turned into questions such as: 1) In what sense is scientific knowledge tentative? In what sense is it durable? etc.”

NATURE OF SCIENCE INSTRUCTION AND MENDEL'S SCIENTIFIC RESEARCH

An historically accurate depiction of Mendel's research, supported by Mendel's own words from his paper, offers an excellent example to students of the nature of science. Mendel's seminal 1866 paper Versuche über Pflanzen-Hybriden (Experiments on plant hybrids) and his letters to Carl Nägeli, can engage students about specific nature of science characteristics and their relationship to four of Mendel's major contributions to genetics. The use of primary source literature as an instructional tool helps students more clearly understand how scientists work and how the science of genetics has evolved as a discipline.

We include explicit nature of science instruction in our genetics courses during the lecture on Gregor Mendel. We do this by discussing selected passages from Mendel's classic 1866 paper to show correlations with NOS-1 through NOS-6. We ask them to examine a poster that we have prepared entitled ‘An understanding of Gregor Mendel's contributions to science from a nature of science perspective’ (Westerlund and Fairbanks 2005). We then discuss the manner in which Gregor Mendel's study exemplifies the six nature of science characteristics. We are able to demonstrate the success of this approach by analyzing the responses of students to open-ended essay questions.

In the following section, we provide a supplemental reading for genetics or biology students that includes a short history of Mendel's scientific research and passages from Mendel's 1866 paper that correlate with NOS-1 through NOS-6. We suggest that instructors provide the reading to their students prior to the traditional Mendel lecture. The reading correlates each of the six NOS characteristics with Mendel's scientific research. After an adequate time for student reflection on the reading, instructors may choose to hold a class discussion based upon questions conducive to effective nature of science instruction as mentioned earlier. Using this approach, the time-honored lecture topic concerning the work of Gregor Mendel, can be quite useful in illustrating the “nature and development of scientific knowledge.”

STUDENT READING- CORRELATION OF GREGOR MENDEL'S DISCOVERIES TO THE NATURE OF SCIENCE

The nature of science [NOS] concerns the practice of science and the development of scientific knowledge. This reading guides you through a study of Gregor Mendel's discoveries in heredity correlated to six major characteristics of the nature of science [NOS-1 through NOS-6]. An example of each NOS characteristic is presented and noted in tables throughout the reading.

Over an eight-year period (1856–1863), Gregor Mendel conducted a series of extensive hybridization experiments with the garden pea, Pisum sativum, in his monastery garden and greenhouse. From the results of these experiments and his interpretations of them, he developed a theory to describe the fundamental mechanisms of heredity. He is recognized for his creative genius as being the first scientist to bring together three different disciplines in biological experimentation: mathematics, the fertilization of gametes and probability theory, to investigate heredity (Orel and Hartl 1994). For his theory, which became the foundation of hereditary principles, Mendel is commonly referred to as the founder of genetics. Mendel described a wide range of genetic phenomena in his paper, including plant reproduction, artificial cross-pollination, segregation, independent assortment, dominance, parental equivalence, pleiotropy, epistasis, the role of hybridization in speciation, and statistical analysis of genetic data. However, his so-called laws of segregation and independent assortment are his most cited discoveries. According to the National Academy of Science, (NAS), a scientific law is defined as “a descriptive generalization about how some aspect of the natural world behaves under stated circumstances,” and a theory as “a well-substantiated explanation of some aspect of the natural world that can incorporate facts, laws, inferences, and tested hypotheses” (NAS 2008). According to these definitions, segregation and independent assortment, as described by Mendel, may be stated as laws. Mendel himself referred to his discoveries as the “law of development” (Entwicklungs-Gesetz) and the “law of combination” (Gesetz der Combinirung). Moreover, his interpretation and explanations of those laws, which have been substantially confirmed and augmented, qualify as the fundamental theory of heredity (Table 1).

NOS-1 Scientific knowledge may be considered as scientific laws or theories according to the type of knowledge (i.e. mathematical and/or explanatory).Mendel's law of segregation
Mendel's law of independent assortment
Fundamental theory of heredity.

Carl Correns (1900), Hugo de Vries (1900a, 1900b) and Erich Tschermak (1900), rediscovered Mendel's law of segregation in 1900, and Correns, in the same 1900 paper, rediscovered the law of independent assortment (Fairbanks and Rytting 2001). Before Mendel, the mechanisms by which traits are inherited were incorrectly or vaguely explained. Mendel presented abundant experimental evidence and offered a well-supported theoretical framework to explain the patterns of inheritance that he observed in a detailed and unambiguous way. Mendel did not title his laws as “segregation” and “independent assortment” as textbooks typically describe them; instead various biologists attributed them to him (Fairbanks and Rytting 2001; Orel and Hartl 1994). For example, de Vries (1900a, 1900b) titled his rediscovery papers, published separately in German and French, “The law of segregation of hybrids” (Das Spaltungsgesetz der Bastarde, and Sur la loi de disjonction des hybrides). However, as several authors have pointed out (Orel and Hartl 1994; Orel 1996; Fairbanks and Rytting 2001; Franklin et al. 2008), and as we will reiterate momentarily, Mendel clearly stated these laws in his paper.

Mendel's development of his laws can be correlated to the nature of science characteristics. For example, NOS-2 is clearly evident from his observations of the different phenotypes of Pisum sativum growing in his experimental gardens (Table 2).

NOS-2 Scientific knowledge is based upon evidence from observations of the natural world.Observations of Pisum sativum, the garden pea in the experimental gardens.

Another example, NOS-3, is illustrated in his paper by the manner in which Mendel interpreted the results of his experiments using the terminology and the available technology of the 1850s; primarily true-breeding pea varieties and cross-fertilization techniques (Table 3).

NOS-3 Scientific knowledge is embedded in the culture in which science is conducted.Mendel's design and interpretation of the results of his experiments.

Furthermore, Mendel's decision to conduct experiments on Pisum sativum were based upon the considerable discussion in the scientific literature of his time regarding heredity and the role of hybridization in the development of new species (Roberts 1965). The problem of not being able to predict offspring from sheep breeding in the region where Mendel lived had “serious economic implications that was crying out for experimental investigations” (Orel 1996, p. 32). The Sheep Breeding Society of Brno in the 1830s was very active in encouraging further study of the problem of heredity (Orel 1996). NOS-3 is also illustrated by the scientific research questions that Mendel posed about heredity were embedded in his culture.

We now examine the development of four of Mendel's discoveries from a nature of science perspective. These discoveries, formulated in modern terminology, include: 1) inheritance of traits is particulate rather than blended; 2) for every diploid organism, inherited traits are governed by genes, and each gene has a pair of alleles that influence variation; 3) paired alleles segregate during the formation of gametes (Mendel's law of segregation) and 4) alleles of different genes assort independently during the formation of gametes (Mendel's law of independent assortment).

The idea of blending inheritance derives from the observations that hybrids often display a phenotype intermediate between their two parents, and that the intermediate hybrid phenotype should be maintained in the progeny of hybrids, implying that the hereditary material of both parents is somehow blended in the hybrid. However, this notion of blending inheritance was already in dispute when Mendel began his experiments. Although some researchers had claimed observation of an intermediate phenotype being maintained in the offspring of hybrids, as blending inheritance predicts, numerous published experiments, which Mendel had studied extensively, argued otherwise. At the outset of Versuche, Mendel specifically mentioned the work of “Kölreuter, Gärtner, Herbert, Lecoq, Wichura and others” (Stern and Sherwood 1966, p. 1). A perusal of the experimental results of these researchers, as summarized by Roberts (1965), shows, as Mendel noted, that hybrids do not always display a phenotype that is exactly intermediate between their parents, and the offspring of hybrids tend be highly variable. Darwin likewise mentioned in ‘The Origin of Species’ that “the slight degree of variability in hybrids from the first cross or in the first generation, in contrast with their extreme variability in the succeeding generations, is a curious fact and deserves attention” (Darwin 1861, p. 296), a passage that Mendel marked with double lines in his personal copy of ‘The Origin of Species’ (Orel 1996; Fairbanks and Rytting 2001).

Although Mendel is often credited as being the first to dispel the notion of blending inheritance, it is clear that others had already done so. For example, Charles Naudin received the 1861 grand prize in physical sciences from the Paris Academy for Sciences for his studies of plant hybrids and their offspring. The Academy noted in its review that Naudin “has confirmed that which Sageret already knew, that in a hybrid the characters of the two parents are often shown not blended” (Roberts 1965, p. 130). Following Naudin, Darwin elaborated in his two-volume work, ‘The Variation of Plants and Animals under Domestication’:

“As a general rule, crossed offspring in the first generation are nearly intermediate between their parents, but the grandchildren and succeeding generations continually revert, in a greater or lesser degree, to one or both of their progenitors. Several authors have maintained that hybrids and mongrels included all the characters of both parents, not fused together, but merely mingled in different proportions in different parts of the body, or as Naudin has expressed it, a hybrid is a living mosaic-work, in which the eye cannot distinguish the discordant elements, so completely are they intermingled.

Darwin 1868, vol. 2, pp. 48–49

Mendel recognized his contribution as uniquely augmenting and explaining a concept already well studied in his day – the uniformity of hybrids compared with the variability of their offspring. In his words, “among the numerous experiments not one has been carried out to an extent or in a manner that would make it possible to determine the number of different forms in which hybrid progeny appear, permit classification of these forms in each generation with certainty, and ascertain their numerical interrelationships” (Stern and Sherwood 1966, p. 2). In other words, Mendel perceived his work as breaking new ground by offering an experimentally supported and mathematically coherent model that explained how variation in the offspring of hybrids was transmitted through inheritance. Although Mendel's work was clearly new, and was the first to deduce in detail the fundamental principles of inheritance, it fit, as Olby (1979, pp. 53–54) put it, “squarely within the context of mid-nineteenth century biology.”

From the historical evidence, we see in Mendel's work an example of NOS-4 wherein Mendel's science was influenced by his readings in current scientific literature and the scientific context of his time (Table 4).

NOS-4 Scientific knowledge is developed within prevailing scientific concepts (theory-laden observations and interpretations) and within scientists’ values, knowledge and prior experiences.Mendel believed that inheritance of traits is discrete, rather than blended from studying the work of his predecessors and designed his experiments based upon it.

He doubted previously held notions concerning blending inheritance and designed his research to augment the work of his predecessors. The design of his experiments and the interpretation of his data were influenced by the scientific thinking of his time.

Mendel realized that one pollen grain must fertilize one ovule. This was a new and controversial idea in germ cell theory during Mendel's time (Orel 1996, p. 194). Mendel understood that development is

“initiated by the union of one germinal cell and one pollen cell into one single cell, which is able to develop into an independent organism through incorporation of matter and the formation of new cells. This development proceeds in accord with a constant law based on the material composition and arrangement of the elements [Mendel's term for genes] that attained a viable union in the cell”

Stern and Sherwood 1966, p. 41–42

Mendel had studied plant physiology with Professor Franz Unger at the University of Vienna, who introduced Mendel to the concept of one pollen grain fertilizing one ovule. In this, we see another example of NOS-4 in the development of ideas in a scientist as deriving from his educational background. Mendel even conducted experiments with Mirabilis to specifically demonstrate that a single pollen grain is sufficient to fertilize an ovule (Stern and Sherwood 1966; Orel 1996). Mendel's understanding that a fertilized germ cell arises from the mutual and equal contribution of a pair of parental units set the stage for his further understanding that an outward phenotypic variation is a result of variation for a pair of alleles, and that 3:1 ratios occur in the F2 offspring as a result.

Gregor Mendel is generally remembered for his work with peas and his discovery of the 3:1 ratio of traits exhibited by hybrids. His recognition of a 3:1 ratio of dominant to recessive phenotypes in the F2 generation, and the theoretical implications derived from this observation, was a seminal achievement in his analyses that set him apart from his contemporaries (Orel 1996). For example, his contemporary Charles Darwin crossed the peloric snapdragon with the common form and allowed the F1 offspring to self-fertilize. The results from this experiment yielded F2 offspring – 88 in the common and 37 in the peloric form – a ratio of 2.38:1, which approaches a 3:1 ratio. However, Darwin apparently did not notice the significance of this ratio (Darwin 1868, vol. 11, p. 70), and did not continue the experiment into the F3 generation. Perhaps Darwin's preconceptions concerning multiple pollen fertilization of an ovule and mosaicism compromised his ability to recognize the significance of the ratio he observed in the F2 offspring. Or, since Darwin did not examine a large series of experiments as Mendel did, he may not have observed a consistent and repeated pattern that demanded an explanation.

Mendel's insight from his formal studies concerning germ cell theory and discrete inheritance, allowed him to recognize not only the significance of the F2 phenotypic 3:1 ratio but also to see that this 3:1 ratio is a consequence of a genotypic ratio of 1:2:1, in other words, 1/4 homozygous dominant, 2/4 heterozygous, and 1/4 homozygous recessive. The 1:2:1 genotypic ratio meant that of the plants in the F2 generation with the dominant phenotype, 1/3 should be homozyous and 2/3 heterozygous, and all of the recessive F2 individuals were homozygous.

To explain his consistent 3:1 ratios in the F2 offspring, Mendel did something different from his contemporaries and very creative, which is an example of NOS-5: he allowed the F2 offspring to naturally self-fertilize and thus was able to resolve the phenotypic 3:1 ratio into the genotypic 1:2:1 ratio. As Mendel stated,

“The ratio of 3:1 in which the distribution of the dominating trait and recessive traits takes place in the first generation therefore resolves itself into the ratio of 2:1:1 in all experiments if one differentiates between the meaning of the dominating trait as a hybrid trait and as a parental character.”

Stern and Sherwood 1966, p. 15

Mendel presented his ideas in mathematical terms and used the designation of Aa to indicate a heterozygote and A or a to indicate a homozygote. Mendel used notation resembling fractions to symbolize the contributions of hereditary elements in the pollen and ovule cells. He represented elements contributed by pollen cells in the numerator and those in ovule cells in the denominator. This notation indicates how Mendel “placed the average course of fertilization in the context of the simple series” (Orel 1996, p. 115).

Stern and Sherwood 1966, p. 30

Mendel's decision to allow the F2 offspring to self-fertilize so that he could identify the 1:2:1 genotypic ratio, and his decision to apply mathematics to his theory clearly illustrate that his scientific knowledge originated from imaginative and creative processes (Table 5).

NOS-5, Scientific knowledge originates from imaginative and creative processes.Mendel's decision to allow the F2 offspring to self-fertilize and to apply mathematics to his theory.

Mendel's laws of segregation and independent assortment are invariably stated in the chapter on Mendelian genetics in general biology and genetics textbooks. For example, Mendel's law of segregation may be stated as, “the two alleles for each trait separate (segregate) during gamete formation” (Hartwell et al. 2004, p. 20), and independent assortment as during gamete formation, different pairs of alleles segregate independently of each other” (Hartwell et al. 2004, p. 26). Although Mendel did not state these laws in the same way as they are now stated, he did state them quite clearly in several passages using the terminology of his day, as in the following, which is his statement of the law of segregation:

“One could perhaps assume that in those hybrids [heterozygotes] whose offspring are variable a compromise takes place between the differing elements of the germinal and pollen cell great enough to permit the formation of a cell that becomes the basis for the hybrid, but that this balance between antagonistic elements is only temporary and does not extend beyond the lifetime of the hybrid plant. Since no changes in its characteristics can be noticed throughout the vegetative period, we must further conclude that the differing elements succeed in escaping from the enforced association only at the stage at which the reproductive cells develop. In the formation of these cells, all elements present participate in completely free and uniform fashion, and only those that differ separate from each other. In this manner the production of as many kinds of germinal and pollen cells would be possible as there are combinations of potentially formative elements.”

Stern and Sherwood 1966, pp. 42–43 (italics in the original)

Mendel's “potentially formative elements” were in today's terminology “genes or alleles,” and inherited determiners of traits carried within the pollen and egg cells. His reference to “the differing elements succeed in escaping from the enforced association only at the stage at which the reproductive cells develop” demonstrates his understanding that the different alleles (A and a) are paired in heterozygotes (Aa). He viewed the elements as being paired until they separate from one another when “the reproductive cells develop,” This is a clear description of segregation of paired alleles during meiosis, or Mendel's law of segregation.

Mendel also quite clearly stated the principle of independent assortment in his paper, albeit not with the same terms commonly used today. After summarizing the results of his dihybrid and trihybrid experiments, Mendel concluded the section stating this law as, “the behavior of each pair of differing traits in a hybrid association is independent of all other differences in the two parental plants” (Stern and Sherwood 1966, p. 22, italics in the original).

Mendel's laws are durable in that they have been repeatedly demonstrated over a period of more than a century in numerous species, although they have been modified on the basis of further evidence (NOS-6). For example, our understanding today of segregation is that alleles in both the heterozygous and homozygous states separate during anaphase I of meiosis. That alleles in both the heterozygous and homozygous states separate from their “enforced association” is not indicated in Mendel's paper. Mendel viewed segregation as being restricted to heterozygotes (Fairbanks and Rytting 2001). NOS-6 is clearly evident in this example that scientific knowledge, (the segregation of alleles) is durable and can be modified (now includes alleles in the homozygous state) with further evidence (Table 6).

NOS-6 Scientific knowledge is not dogmatic but considered durable and can be modified or replaced with further evidenceMendel viewed segregation of alleles as being restricted to heterozygotes, which has been modified to include homozygotes

Moreover, the discoveries that genes are located on chromosomes, and that unlinked genes assort independently whereas linked genes do not is an important modification of Mendel's law of independent assortment.

Mendel demonstrated his laws of segregation and independent assortment mathematically, but ventured beyond mathematical demonstration to propose an explanatory theory of inheritance based on the cell theory of his day (Table 1). As stated earlier, this is an example of NOS-1, that concerns the difference between scientific theories and laws. His theory of inheritance remains durable, an example of (NOS-6), as the modern foundational theory of inheritance.

End of Student Reading

SUMMARY

We have used a nature of science perspective in our analysis of Mendel's experiments to explore his contributions to genetics. In our genetics courses, we have discussed the nature of science in relation to Mendel's discoveries. We present it here as a way for genetics instructors to enrich and expand their traditional presentation on Mendel by including characteristics of the nature of science, with references to original source material and accurate historical context. As McComas (2008, p. 261) stressed, “If students do not have an explicit opportunity to link the historical example with an NOS principle [characteristic], they will likely hear these accounts of science and consider them interesting but not particularly enlightening stories.” Thus, students need explicit instruction on the nature of science, and opportunities through questioning to reflect upon them. We suggest that instructors ask their students questions about the nature of science based upon the provided student supplemental reading so that they have opportunities to reflect upon and fully engage in the material. Instructors also may find Clough's (2007) nature-of-science questions beneficial for discussions. These questions can accompany any historical vignette, including Mendel's story and stories about other geneticists (e.g. Bateson, Morgan, Fisher, McClintock, and others) in later lectures. These questions should encourage classroom discussions that are needed to create meaningful learning about the nature of science.

Ancillary

Article Information

DOI

10.1111/j.1601-5223.2010.02199.x

Format Available

Full text: HTML | PDF

© 2010 The Authors

Publication History

  • Issue online:
  • Version of record online:
  • Paper received 14 June 2010. manuscript accepted 6 September 2010.

REFERENCES

  • Abd-El-Khalick F. and Lederman N. G. 2000. Improving science teachers’ conceptions of nature of science: a critical review of the literature. Int. J. Sci. Educ.22: 665–701.
  • Abd-El-Khalick F., Bell R. L. and Lederman N. G. 1998. The nature of science and instructional practice: making the unnatural natural. Sci. Educ.82: 417–436.
  • Alberts B. 2009. Redefining science education. Science323: 437.
  • Alters B. 1997. Whose nature of science?J. Res. Sci. Teach.34: 39–55.
  • AAAS1989. Science for all Americans, chapter one, [online] <www.project2061.org/publications/sfaa/default.htm> accessed 26 May 2010.
  • AAAS2009. Conference mobilizes educators to transform undergraduate biology education. <www.aaas.org/news/releases/2009/0821biology_conference.shtml> accessed 20 February 2010.
  • Bartholomew H., Osborne J. and Ratcliffe M. 2004. Teaching students ideas about science: five dimensions of effective practice. Sci. Educ.88: 655–682.

0 comments

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *